1.IntroductionMotion can distort the position perception of objects. Many mislocalizations are accompanied by existence of a moving visual stimulus.1–3 One of the best-known and most-studied motion-induced mislocalizations is the flash-lag effect (FLE),4–6 where a flash would be perceived as lagging behind a moving object even if they are actually aligned. In addition to the classical FLE, Schlag et al.7 found an extension of the FLE without stimulation of visual motion: a flash would be perceived as lagging behind an object that remains in one spot relative to the head when the observer is making head rotations. To account for this novel head-rotation-induced FLE (herein abbreviated as hFLE), Schlag et al. followed the “motion extrapolation” hypothesis proposed by Nijhawan5 that the neural system tends to predict the position of a moving object to compensate for the delay caused by the transmission and processing of neural signals. Although there was no retinal motion in this hFLE, Schlag et al. proposed that extraretinal motion could also be extrapolated. As to the neural origin for the FLE, Maus et al.8 and Wang et al.9 proved the causal role of human middle temporal complex (hMT+) in FLE with TMS and tDCS, respectively. It is well known that hMT+ plays an essential role in forming the perception of visual motion and object position. This area not only represents the physical motion or position but also encodes implied motion, such as the motion aftereffect,10–14 and the perceived position of the observer.15,16 Thus, the hMT+ is involved in many mislocalizations induced by motion including the FLE.17–19 By contrast, to our knowledge, the brain activity in the hFLE has not been investigated. As a multisensory illusion, it can be foreseen that the hFLE involves even more complicated processing than its classical visual version, but still surprisingly little is known about it. Elucidating the mechanism underlying the hFLE may shed light on the complex visual-vestibular interactions that occur every day in natural viewing conditions. Furthermore, it will add to our knowledge of the vestibular cognition, which, differing from other sensory modalities, is extensively distributed across the brain with distinct physiological characteristics and awaits deeper explorations.20–23 In previous work, we pointed out that the motion extrapolation hypothesis could not provide an adequate explanation of the forming mechanism of hFLE.24 Instead, the account of visual-vestibular interaction was more preferred. We notice that vestibular stimulations25–27 and visual-vestibular integration28 can also activate hMT+. Moreover, hMT+ is functionally connected to several regions in the vestibular cortical network.29,30 Considering the contribution of hMT+ to the classical FLE and the functions of hMT+ concerning both motion processing and visual-vestibular integration, we hypothesized that hMT+ would be activated when observer experienced the hFLE. To test this hypothesis, one should seek an appropriate neuroimaging tool first. Unfortunately, this is considerably challenging owing to the technical limitations for common tools (e.g., fMRI and EEG). Compared with fMRI and EEG that are both susceptible to motion artifacts, functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) is more tolerant to motion and has thus been broadly employed in research on exercises, under natural scenes, or with special participants like infants. In this study, we measured with fNIRS the cortical activity around hMT+ of the participants who performed horizontal head rotations to experience the hFLE. 2.Methods2.1.ParticipantsTwenty-eight participants (11 males and 17 females) were recruited for the research, whose ages ranged from 19 to 32 years ( years). The sample size was determined using G*Power31 after a pilot experiment with 11 participants. All the participants had normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity and reported no history of motion sickness [including simulator sickness with VR or three-dimensional (3D) display] nor dysfunction of visual or vestibular system. This study protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences. All the participants gave their informed consent and were paid after the experiment. 2.2.ApparatusThe experiment program was written and run with MATLAB R2013a (MathWorks Inc.) and Psychtoolbox v3.0.1232 on a Dell XPS 8700 PC (Dell Inc.), and the visual stimuli were presented to the participants on a Sony HMZ-T3 head-mounted goggle (Sony, Japan) with a field of view of 50 (horizontal) × 28 (vertical) degrees (deg) of visual angle, resolution of , and refresh rate of 60 Hz. The goggles were worn outside the fNIRS headcap. Participants sat on a barber’s chair to achieve rotation with their feet off the ground, and a TSS-WL 3-Space wireless motion sensor (Yost Labs Inc.) was fixed to one arm of the chair. The fNIRS recording was implemented by a LABNIRS continuous-wave fNIRS system (Shimadzu Inc., Japan) with the near-infrared wavelengths of 780, 805, and 830 mm. Optode localization was carried out with a Fastrak 3D tracking and digitizing system (Polhemus Inc.). 2.3.fNIRS AcquisitionThe optodes were arranged around T5/T6 of the international 10–20 system to cover bilateral temporo-parieto-occipital areas.33–35 On each side was a square layout with five emitters and four detectors yielding 12 channels [Fig. 1(a)]. The raw sampling rate of fNIRS data was 55.6 Hz. After each participant completed the main task, a localization was carried out for each optode as well as for anatomical landmarks of the vertex (Cz), nasion (Nz), and bilateral preauricular points (AL/AR). These localization data were then used to estimate the locations of the channels on the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) brain template.36–38 The average locations of the channels on the brain template are shown in Fig. 1(b). The cortical subdivisions they fell on were then estimated based on the LONI Probabilistic Brain Atlas (LPBA40)39 and Brodmann areas (BA). 2.4.Stimuli and ProcedureWe assumed that the relevant brain activation during the hFLE was the result of highly independent components caused by three types of neural signals, namely: (i) response to visual stimulus (i.e., the flash bar), (ii) response to vestibular stimulus (i.e., the horizontal body rotation), and (iii) the signal of visual–vestibular interaction leading to the hFLE. Based on this assumption, we designed five experimental conditions to decompose the brain activation during the hFLE. 2.4.1.Short flash + moving (sF+M)In light of previous findings where passive movement could also induce the hFLE,40 in this study, participants were seated in a swivel barber’s chair with their feet off the ground. The horizontal rotation was performed by the experimenter to reduce the influence of cervical movement on the fNIRS recording. The experimenter received a tone cue through earphones to start or stop the rotation. The display on the LCD monitor was identical to that on the goggles, allowing the experimenter to read the instruction. Throughout the whole experimental session, the background of the display was pure black with a centered, red fixation point with 0.15 deg diameter. Before each block started, two lines of texts displayed the number of upcoming blocks and an instruction “Rotate!” to remind participants that they needed to rotate during the block. After participants were ready with their feet off the ground, the experimenter pressed a key to start the block. For the first 5 s, only a central reference bar and the fixation point were presented while a countdown with beep tones through earphones reminded the experimenter to prepare to rotate the chair. The reference bar was white with a size of 5 deg (height) × 0.5 deg (width). The experimenter rotated the chair once per trial based on the auditory cues from the earphones. A high tone (1500 Hz) indicated starting the rotation, and a low tone (1000 Hz) indicated ceasing, each lasting for 50 ms. Each rotation lasted for 2 s with 1-s intertrial intervals (Fig. 2). The experimenter purposely controlled the ends of rotation trajectories on approximately the same positions to keep the rotation amplitude roughly constant across trials. Each block consisted of 15 trials with alternating rotating directions between leftward and rightward. After a random delay ranging from 0.6 to 1.2 s starting from the beginning of each rotation, a flash bar identical to the reference bar would appear 5 deg right above the reference bar for 1 frame (16.7 ms) and then vanish. Although the flash bar and target bar were vertically aligned physically, the participant was informed in advance that there was always a slight misalignment between the two bars and required to perform a 2AFC task discriminating whether the flash bar was to the left or the right of the reference bar by pressing the left or the right arrow key while maintaining fixation at the central fixation point. The reference bar remained centered on the display throughout each block. The mean velocity across 0.5 s right before each flash onset was recorded as the rotation velocity. The average rotation velocities for conditions with rotation (i.e., conditions A, C, and E) are listed in Table 1. Table 1Summary of all conditions.
Note: Rotation velocities are reported in mean ± standard deviation. The velocity in condition C was lower than that in condition A because of their different flash onset delays relative to the beginning of trials (see Sec. 2.4). 2.4.2.Short flash + static (sF+S)Condition B was designed to measure the brain activation caused by the visual stimulus alone in condition A. Therefore, participants in condition B were not rotated. The other procedures and tasks were the same as those in condition A, except that the instruction before the block read “Do NOT rotate!” instead. 2.4.3.Long flash + moving (lF+M)Past studies have shown that the flash-lag effect may weaken with increasing duration of the flash stimulus.41–44 Furthermore, the upper limit of the flash duration that could induce the FLE was 80 to 500 ms.41,42,44,45 Thus, in condition C we lengthened the duration of the flash bar to 1 s. Hopefully, this modification might suppress the hFLE while maintaining similar visual stimuli to those in condition A. In terms of timing, the random delay before the flash was altered to 0.3 to 0.8 s (Fig. 2). Note that the rotation velocity recorded in condition C was lower than that in condition A (see Table 1) due to this shorter delay. The other procedures and tasks were identical to those in condition A, and participants were required to make responses after the flash bar disappeared. 2.4.4.Long flash + static (lF+S)Condition D was almost the same as condition C except that participants in this condition were not rotated. The instruction before the block also read “Do NOT rotate!” instead of “Rotate!” 2.4.5.No flash + moving (nF+M)Condition E served as a control to measure the brain activation caused solely by the self-rotation. Therefore, the flash bar was not presented and participants did not have to make any response. All the participant had to do was to keep gazing at the fixation point. The other procedures were the same as those in condition A. In particular, the program sent a trigger to the LABNIRS system to mark a virtual “flash-onset” event at a random time point of each trial to maintain a consistent data structure with the other conditions for the fNIRS data analyses. The conditions are summarized and compared in Fig. 3 and Table 1. Each condition consisted of five blocks, resulting in a total of 25 blocks for each participant, with the order randomized according to a Latin square design. Participants received a minimum of 15-s rest between blocks during which they could take a break without large head or body movements. 2.5.Behavioral Data AnalysisFor conditions A and C, the proportion of responses that were in the opposite direction to self-rotation (e.g., reporting the flash bar to the left of the reference bar when rotating rightward and vice versa) was calculated to indicate the intensity of hFLE. If this proportion was significantly higher than 50%, it would indicate an hFLE. As for conditions B and D where the participants did not rotate, we simply calculated the proportion of responses reporting the flash bar to the left to measure response bias. A repeated-measures ANOVA (short/long flash × moving/static) was then conducted on these response proportions to compare the intensity of hFLE across different conditions. 2.6.fNIRS Data AnalysisBoth the absorbance (ABS) data of each wavelength and the relative changes in concentration of oxygenated, deoxygenated, and total hemoglobin (denoted as [HbO], [HbR], and [tHb], respectively) were obtained from the fNIRS system. These data were processed and analyzed using MATLAB 2017a (MathWorks Inc.) with the NIRS-KIT46 and SPM1247 packages. 2.6.1.Pre-processingThe pre-processing consisted of several steps. Figure 4 demonstrates the pre-processing pipeline step by step with an instance.
2.6.2.First-level (individual) analysisAnalyses below mainly focused on the change of [HbO] as it is considered a more sensitive measure compared to [HbR] or [tHb]56–58 and many past studies have selected [HbO] as the main analysis target.34,59–61 However, the results for [HbR] will also be reported as a comparison. The data were analyzed using a block design approach. The onset of the first flash and the offset of the last one in each block were defined as the beginning and end, respectively, and the duration between them was considered as the block duration. After pre-processed, the data of each participant were fitted to a GLM based on the canonical (two-gamma) hemodynamic response function provided by the SPM12 toolbox to obtain a -value for each condition ( to ). Then, we focused on three types of contrasts below: (i) , (ii) , and (iii) . According to the assumption mentioned above, contained all three types of signals while represented only the effect of the visual stimulus, so represented the vestibular component and the vestibular-visual interacting component (i.e., the hFLE). The case was similar for and , except that the long flash was supposed not to induce the hFLE, so represented only the vestibular component. Therefore, should represent a relatively pure effect of the hFLE. We also calculated by replacing with . And to examine whether the long flash was capable of inducing the hFLE, was also calculated. 2.6.3.Second-level (group) analysisWe conducted one-sample -tests to compare on each channel with a baseline of 0 in order to examine the differential brain activation across conditions. A significant deviation from 0 for or would indicate a potential locus that represented the hFLE on that channel. Besides, we analyzed the correlation between and rotation velocity or behavioral performance to relate neural activation to stimulus intensity or perception. As the analyses were conducted separately on each channel, multiple comparison corrections were implemented using the false discovery rate (FDR) method62 within either hemisphere. For all the statistics above and below, effect sizes are reported as Cohen’s ( for short) for -tests or for ANOVA. All the -values reported in the ANOVA have been adjusted where necessary using Greenhouse–Geisser correction, and post hoc tests were conducted with the Tukey–Kramer method. 3.Results3.1.Behavioral PerformanceAccording to the one-sample -test results (Fig. 5), when the participants remained static (i.e., conditions B and D), their responses for the flash bar appearing on the left side did not deviate significantly from 50% [B: ; D: ; ], indicating little response bias. However, when the participants were moving (conditions A and C), responses for the flash bar appearing in the opposite side of the rotating direction were significantly greater than 50% of the total [A: , , , ; C; , , , ], which suggested an evident hFLE in both flash durations. As for the ANOVA, the main effect of flash duration was significant [, , ] with a higher response proportion for short flashes than for long ones, and so was the main effect of motion state [, , ] with a higher response proportion when moving than when static. The interaction between flash duration and motion state was also significant [, , ]: the response proportion was higher when moving than when static () but only for short flashes. In addition, the response proportion for short flashes was much higher than that for long flashes when moving () but slightly lower when static (). These results showed that although either short or long flashes could induce the hFLE, the effect was relatively weaker for long flashes than for short flashes. 3.2.fNIRSThe actual sample sizes in the fNIRS analyses were less than the number of participants since some channels of some participants were excluded in the pre-processing stage, which will be reported below. First, we examined the deviations of from 0. For [HbO], as shown in Fig. 6 and Table 2, several channels showed significant deviation of from 0, but none of them survived the FDR correction. deviated significantly from 0 on more channels, among which Ch. #3, #10, #20, #22, and #23 remained significant after the FDR correction. Similarly, several channels showed significant , and Ch. #23 passed the FDR correction. In contrast, for [HbR], Ch. #22 showed significant deviation of from 0 and #7, #14, and #22 showed significant deviation of from 0 (all ), but none of them survived the FDR correction. Table 2Results of the examinations on Δβ with uncorrected ps<0.05.
Note: N means the size of the sample actually included in the analysis on each channel, and bold font indicates surviving the FDR correction. We then decided to focus on Ch. #3, #10, #20, and #22, which passed the FDR correction, as the main results. Although Ch. #23 also passed the FDR correction for , it was excluded with caution because it did so for the control as well, which was not specific enough to the hFLE. Due to a similar reason, Ch. #17 and #24 were leniently selected because they showed relatively large raw but no , which was consistent with the anticipation of the experiment. The results of localization for them are listed in Table 3. Table 3Brain regions corresponding to the focused channels.
Note: SMG, supramarginal gyrus; STG, superior temporal gyrus; MOG, middle occipital gyrus; IOG, inferior occipital gyrus; AG, angular gyrus; MTG, middle temporal gyrus; ITG, inferior temporal gyrus. The correlation between and rotation velocity in the two conditions with rotation (i.e., A and C) was then explored. As shown in Fig. 7 and Table 4, Ch. #3 showed significant positive correlation in condition A (, , ) and Ch. #15 in condition C (, , ). Besides, both channels showed the trend of positive correlation on the other condition (Ch. #3, condition C: , , ; Ch. #15, condition A: , , ). In fact, the posterior temporal-parietal area was found to have a slightly higher correlation than other tested regions (Fig. 8). Table 4Channels with correlation to the rotation velocity.
Note: MTG, middle temporal gyrus; ITG, inferior temporal gyrus; AG, angular gyrus; SOG, superior occipital gyrus; MOG, middle occipital gyrus. Finally, we tested the correlation between and the corresponding hFLE intensity in conditions A and C. None of the focused channels were found to have a significant correlation, but Ch. #6 (, , ) and #8 (, ) showed significant negative correlation between and the response proportion in condition A, whereas Ch. #15 (, , ) and #18 (, , ) showed significant positive correlation between and the response proportion in condition C (Fig. 9). However, these results did not pass the FDR correction. The regions involved for these channels are listed in Table 5. As shown in Fig. 10, the negative correlation was mainly distributed across posterior part of the medium temporal gyrus in condition A, whereas the lateral middle occipital area showed positive correlation in condition C. Table 5Channels with correlation to the behavioral performance.
Note: MTG, middle temporal gyrus; ITG, inferior temporal gyrus; AG, angular gyrus; SOG, superior occipital gyrus; MOG, middle occipital gyrus; IOG, inferior occipital gyrus. However, no significant correlation was observed directly between rotation velocity and behavioral performance (condition A: , ; condition C: , ). In summary, the brain activity related to the hFLE was significant in right MTG/ITG (BA 37; Ch. #20 & #24), bilateral SMG/STG (BA 22; Ch. #3 & #17), and bilateral MOG/IOG (BA 18/19; Ch. #10 & #20). The activity in the SMG correlated with the rotation velocity, whereas that in the MTG correlated with the hFLE intensity. 4.DiscussionBy using fNIRS, this study measured for the first time the brain activity corresponding to the hFLE. We provided the neuroimaging evidence for the engagement of left supramarginal gyrus (SMG)/superior temporal gyrus (STG) area (BA 22), the posterior part of bilateral middle temporal gyri (MTG)/inferior temporal gyri (ITG) areas (BA 37), and lateral occipital areas (BA 18/19). To manipulate the hFLE while minimizing the alteration of the visual stimuli, we extended the duration of the flash bar based on previous literature on classical FLE.41,42,44 However, it turned out that the hFLE was reduced but not fully eliminated when the flash duration was extended to 1 s, as shown by the behavioral results that the hFLE for the long flash was also significant though weaker than that for the short flash and not significantly different from the control condition when the participants were static. Although it is difficult to propose an explicit account for this finding with only current data, we believe that it might reflect a certain difference between the mechanisms for the hFLE and classical FLE, which requires further empirical evidence. For example, the formation of hFLE might involve higher-level sensory processing such as multisensory interaction compared to the classical FLE and thus took a longer time span, which could cover a longer flash stimulus. On the other hand, the current behavioral task forced participants to discriminate the mislocalization between two physically aligned bars, resulting in virtually no signal to detect. Therefore, intrinsic neural noise might have a significant impact on perception,63,64 leading to the noticeable individual response biases in both static control conditions (B and D). This could also explain the observed hFLE for the long flash. Consistent with our hypothesis that the hMT+ activates during the hFLE, Ch. #22 and #24 were identified by the analyses of contrasts of interest. Both channels are located in the middle and inferior temporal gyri (MTG and ITG) and both in BA 37. Despite that we did not functionally localize the exact divisions, we could infer from these estimated locations that they are likely situated in the middle temporal (MT) area, which is a main subregion of the hMT+, typically located around the occipital continuation and ascending branch of the inferior temporal sulcus.65–68 The hMT+ and its homolog in the macaque have been well recognized as key areas for processing visual motion information, both in macaques and humans. Interestingly, recent studies have demonstrated that they also to respond to vestibular stimuli and participate in visual-vestibular integration to form heading and self-motion perception.25–27,69–74 Della-Justina et al.28 presented participants with visual stimulus of flickering checkboards, vestibular stimulus of galvanic vestibular stimulation (GVS), and combined stimulus, and observed activation of ITG to single visual stimulus and visual-vestibular combined stimulus. Furthermore, Indovina et al.30 used graph-theoretical network analysis to find that BA 37 was structurally connected to the posterior insula cortex (PIC), a key hub in the vestibular cortical network, and that BA 37 itself also showed some hubness in the vestibular cross-modal network. Based on these previous findings, we reckon that the activity of BA 37 is related to visual-vestibular integration processing that occurs in hMT+. The negative correlation between the activity in BA 37 and the behavioral performance may help clarify its role. Given that there was no visual motion stimulus in this study, it is likely that the activity of hMT+ represented the afferent retinal motion signals generated by eye movements and intrinsic neural noises of the participant. One possible explanation for this is that weaker activation of the hMT+ in a participant reflects fewer eye movements or neural intrinsic noises, resulting in weaker feedforward visual signals. This would lead to less weight of visual signals in the visual-vestibular integration and ultimately leave the estimation of object position more susceptible to the modulation of vestibular signals. Alternatively, the hMT+ could be subject to the inhibition by vestibular signals or by the feedback from visual-vestibular integration. Indeed, several past studies have observed mutual inhibition in visual-vestibular integration,29,75,76 especially when the stimulus from the suppressed modality was weak. This suppression might aim to reduce neural noise from the weak modality and resolve sensory conflict.75 In any case, the intensity of hFLE reflects the relative disadvantage of the visual modality compared to the vestibular modality in their integration during self-motion in our study. On the other hand, Ch. #3 and #17 (and #14 in the correlation analysis) are located around SMG and STG (especially the posterior part, BA 22), two areas that also participate in visual-vestibular integration, especially the representation and processing of sensory conflicts.27,33,77 Both areas are parts of the temporoparietal junction (TPJ), a functional complex between the parietal and temporal lobes and around the posterior end of the Sylvian fissure that plays a critical role in human vestibular system (see Ref. 78 for review) and found to be functionally connected to PIC and the parietal-insular vestibular cortex, two essential hubs in the vestibular network.30,79–81 Previous neuroimaging research studies have found that the temporoparietal areas, including the SMG and STG, respond to caloric vestibular stimulation or GVS22,82 and activate in body-related tasks such as standing balance or inner verticality where the cues from other sensory modalities than vestibular were deprived,83,84 and the STG response increased with the GVS intensity.82 In an fNIRS study using horizontal rotation as the vestibular stimulation, Nguyen et al.33 found that TPJ was sensitive to the incongruency between visual and vestibular stimuli, and that the activation of dorsal SMG negatively correlated with the intensity of subjective vertigo reported by the participant. Although both hMT+ and TPJ engage in visual-vestibular integration, there are still some differences. Compared with hMT+, which is more of a visual related area, TPJ is closer to vestibular processing in visual-vestibular integration. This could also explain the different correlation results. The brain activation to the hFLE on Ch. #3 and #14 (TPJ) was positively correlated to the velocity probably because TPJ was more active in solving bigger visual-vestibular conflicts when the vestibular stimulation was stronger. In contrast, the brain activity on Ch. #6 and #8 identified in the correlation analysis on and located in hMT+ influenced behavioral responses, which reflected the visual perception and subsequent sensory decision making affected by the visual-vestibular interaction. As to Ch. #10 and #20 (and #15 and #18 in the correlation analysis), they fell in the lateral part of the medial occipital area (BA 18/19, extrastriate visual cortex V2/V3). The brain activity here might be reflective of the remaining effect from the visual stimulus of the flash, which was not fully eliminated in and . This also explains the two distinct types of correlations between and the hFLE intensity in conditions A and C: in condition A with a weak visual input and strong hFLE, the performance was mainly determined by the cross-modal interaction in hMT+ as stated above, whereas in condition C where the visual input was relatively strong and the illusion was weak, the perception was likely influenced much by the response bias, which reflected the intrinsic neural noise of the extrastriate visual cortex V2/V3, as discussed at the beginning of the section. Finally, despite that the brain activity was observed to correlate with both the rotation velocity and the hFLE intensity, no significant correlation was found directly between the latter two, which was consistent with our previous findings.24 In that study, an hFLE was significantly induced both when the participants actively rotated the head (experiment 1, HM condition) and when they were passively rotated in the chair by the experimenter (experiment 1, BM condition), but the hFLE intensity significantly correlated to the rotation velocity only in the HM and not in the BM condition, which is interesting especially because these two conditions did not show significant difference in the hFLE intensity. And that finding in the BM condition was replicated in this study. We suspect that the decisive process to cause hFLE depends solely on the vestibular stimulation and does not distinguish its source, i.e., whether it comes from an active or a passive self-motion, but the intensity of the hFLE might be modulated by some mechanisms regarding this source, such as corollary discharge.85–87 Note that our findings were not symmetric across both hemispheres, with activation in the right hemisphere being generally stronger. This might be attributed to the fluctuations and noise in neural activity and data acquisition, but another possibility could be lateralization. Many studies have shown that at least part of the vestibular network was lateralized both anatomically and functionally toward a right-sided dominance,23,29,30,88–90 including when processing horizontal rotating directions.91 This lateral dominance was proved dependent partly on the handedness and often showed in the non-dominant hemisphere,81,92 which might account for the current findings, but other factors and mechanisms are also involved.22,30,88,93 An important concern awaiting future investigations is to detail the distinct division of labor in the contribution of these brain regions to the hFLE as well as other visual-vestibular interaction instances, considering the widespread distribution of the vestibular network across the cortex.21,23,94–96 These different roles may even relate to unique physiological characteristics such as elongated hemodynamics response.22 And the current results should be interpreted cautiously and further evidence is still needed due to several limitations. First, the spatial resolution of fNIRS is much lower than fMRI, and fNIRS can only detect cortical surface activation due to its imaging principle. Therefore, the results may not be sufficiently accurate or complete. Besides, the brain atlas used here is mainly anatomical rather than functional, making it challenging to match the current findings to past fMRI research about visual-vestibular integration. Future studies can use neuronavigation techniques to help access more precise ROIs and improve data reliability97–99 and neuromodulation tools such as TMS to confirm whether these areas play a causal role. Also, a motion platform100 can be used to implement finer and stricter manipulations of the motion profiles. 5.ConclusionThis study observed the activation of right SMG/STG area (BA 22), bilateral MTG/ITG areas (BA 37), and bilateral MOG/IOG areas (BA 18/19) in participants experiencing the hFLE. The activation of SMG was positively correlated with the rotation velocity while the activation of MTG was negatively correlated with the intensity of the hFLE. These findings demonstrate the role of visual-vestibular interaction in the formation of hFLE and provide indirect support to the visual-vestibular interaction account of the hFLE,24 suggesting that multiple visual-vestibular interactions help form the hFLE including the processing of multisensory conflicts in TPJ and the biasing of position perception by vestibular information in hMT+. Another minor but intriguing observation was that the hFLE could tolerate a longer duration of the flash stimulus compared to the classical visual FLE, indicating distinct mechanisms of visual processing between these two phenomena. This study also lends more support to the application of fNIRS in multisensory neuroimaging investigations that allows vestibular stimulations of real motion. Code and Data AvailabilityThe data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request. AcknowledgmentsWe would like to appreciate the technical supports of Dr. Chuguang Wei and all other staff at the Institutional Center for Shared Technologies and Facilities of Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, and the kind help from Dr. Lihong Chen, Dr. Xue Dong, and Dr. Zhenghan Li. This research was supported by the Ministry of Science and Technology of China (2021ZD0203800) and the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31830037, 31871104, and 32300878). ReferencesV. S. Ramachandran and S. M. Anstis,
“Illusory displacement of equiluminous kinetic edges,”
Perception, 19
(5), 611
–616 https://doi.org/10.1068/p190611 PCTNBA 0301-0066
(1990).
Google Scholar
J. Müsseler, S. Stork and D. Kerzel,
“Comparing mislocalizations with moving stimuli: the Fröhlich effect, the flash-lag, and representational momentum,”
Vis. Cognit., 9
(1–2), 120
–138 https://doi.org/10.1080/13506280143000359 1350-6285
(2002).
Google Scholar
X. Dong, J. Bai and M. Bao,
“Robust size illusion produced by expanding and contracting flow fields,”
Vis. Res., 133 87
–94 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2017.01.003
(2017).
Google Scholar
D. M. Mackay,
“Perceptual stability of a stroboscopically lit visual field containing self-luminous objects,”
Nature, 181
(4607), 507
–508 https://doi.org/10.1038/181507a0
(1958).
Google Scholar
R. Nijhawan,
“Motion extrapolation in catching,”
Nature, 370
(6487), 256
–257 https://doi.org/10.1038/370256b0
(1994).
Google Scholar
D. Whitney,
“The influence of visual motion on perceived position,”
Trends Cogn. Sci., 6
(5), 211
–216 https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01887-9 TCSCFK 1364-6613
(2002).
Google Scholar
J. Schlag et al.,
“Extrapolating movement without retinal motion,”
Nature, 403
(6765), 38
–39 https://doi.org/10.1038/47402
(2000).
Google Scholar
G. W. Maus et al.,
“The perceived position of moving objects: transcranial magnetic stimulation of area MT+ reduces the flash-lag effect,”
Cereb. Cortex, 23
(1), 241
–247 https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhs021 53OPAV 1047-3211
(2013).
Google Scholar
W. Wang et al.,
“Facilitation and inhibition effects of anodal and cathodal tDCS over areas MT+ on the flash-lag effect,”
J. Neurophysiol., 128
(1), 239
–248 https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00091.2022 JONEA4 0022-3077
(2022).
Google Scholar
R. B. H. Tootell et al.,
“Visual motion aftereffect in human cortical area MT revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging,”
Nature, 375
(6527), 139
–141 https://doi.org/10.1038/375139a0
(1995).
Google Scholar
H. Théoret et al.,
“Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of human area MT/V5 disrupts perception and storage of the motion aftereffect,”
Neuropsychologia, 40
(13), 2280
–2287 https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00112-4 NUPSA6 0028-3932
(2002).
Google Scholar
M. Castelo-Branco et al.,
“Type of featural attention differentially modulates hMT+ responses to illusory motion after effects,”
J. Neurophysiol., 102
(5), 3016
–3025 https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.90812.2008 JONEA4 0022-3077
(2009).
Google Scholar
H. Hogendoorn and F. A. J. Verstraten,
“Decoding the motion aftereffect in human visual cortex,”
Neuroimage, 82 426
–432 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.034 NEIMEF 1053-8119
(2013).
Google Scholar
K.-Y. Li et al.,
“Cognitive and neural mechanisms of implied motion,”
Prog. Biochem. Biophys., 42
(6), 519
–532 https://doi.org/10.16476/j.pibb.2015.0063 PIBBEV 0898-6711
(2015).
Google Scholar
J. Fischer, N. Spotswood and D. Whitney,
“The emergence of perceived position in the visual system,”
J. Cogn. Neurosci., 23
(1), 119
–136 https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2010.21417 JCONEO 0898-929X
(2011).
Google Scholar
G. W. Maus, J. Fischer and D. Whitney,
“Motion-dependent representation of space in area MT+,”
Neuron, 78
(3), 554
–562 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2013.03.010 NERNET 0896-6273
(2013).
Google Scholar
C. Senior, J. Ward and A. S. David,
“Representational momentum and the brain: an investigation into the functional necessity of V5/MT,”
Vis. Cogn., 9
(1–2), 81
–92 https://doi.org/10.1080/13506280143000331 1350-6285
(2002).
Google Scholar
P. V. McGraw, V. Walsh and B. T. Barrett,
“Motion-sensitive neurones in V5/MT modulate perceived spatial position,”
Curr. Biol., 14
(12), 1090
–1093 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2004.06.028 CUBLE2 0960-9822
(2004).
Google Scholar
D. Whitney et al.,
“Visually guided reaching depends on motion area MT+,”
Cereb. Cortex, 17
(11), 2644
–2649 https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhl172 53OPAV 1047-3211
(2007).
Google Scholar
E. R. Ferrè and P. Haggard,
“Vestibular cognition: state-of-the-art and future directions,”
Cogn. Neuropsychol., 37
(7–8), 413
–420 https://doi.org/10.1080/02643294.2020.1736018 COGNEP 1464-0627
(2020).
Google Scholar
Y. Gu,
“Vestibular signals in primate cortex for self-motion perception,”
Curr. Opin. Neurobiol., 52 10
–17 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2018.04.004 COPUEN 0959-4388
(2018).
Google Scholar
H. T. Karim et al.,
“Neuroimaging to detect cortical projection of vestibular response to caloric stimulation in young and older adults using functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS),”
Neuroimage, 76 1
–10 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.02.061 NEIMEF 1053-8119
(2013).
Google Scholar
M. Dieterich and T. Brandt,
“Functional brain imaging of peripheral and central vestibular disorders,”
Brain, 131
(10), 2538
–2552 https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awn042 BRAIAK 0006-8950
(2008).
Google Scholar
X. He et al.,
“Beyond motion extrapolation: vestibular contribution to head-rotation-induced flash-lag effects,”
Psychol. Res., 86
(7), 2083
–2098 https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-021-01638-8
(2022).
Google Scholar
B. M. Seemungal et al.,
“Vestibular activation differentially modulates human early visual cortex and V5/MT excitability and response entropy,”
Cereb. Cortex, 23
(1), 12
–19 https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr366 53OPAV 1047-3211
(2013).
Google Scholar
F. Aedo-Jury et al.,
“Antero-posterior vs. lateral vestibular input processing in human visual cortex,”
Front. Integr. Neurosci., 14 43 https://doi.org/10.3389/fnint.2020.00043
(2020).
Google Scholar
T. Stephan et al.,
“Functional MRI of galvanic vestibular stimulation with alternating currents at different frequencies,”
Neuroimage, 26
(3), 721
–732 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.049 NEIMEF 1053-8119
(2005).
Google Scholar
H. M. Della-Justina et al.,
“Interaction of brain areas of visual and vestibular simultaneous activity with fMRI,”
Exp. Brain Res., 233
(1), 237
–252 https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-014-4107-6 EXBRAP 0014-4819
(2015).
Google Scholar
R. M. Rühl et al.,
“Functional correlate and delineated connectivity pattern of human motion aftereffect responses substantiate a subjacent visual-vestibular interaction,”
Neuroimage, 174 22
–34 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.02.057 NEIMEF 1053-8119
(2018).
Google Scholar
I. Indovina et al.,
“Structural connectome and connectivity lateralization of the multimodal vestibular cortical network,”
Neuroimage, 222 117247 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117247 NEIMEF 1053-8119
(2020).
Google Scholar
F. Faul et al.,
“G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences,”
Behav. Res. Methods, 39
(2), 175
–191 https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193146
(2007).
Google Scholar
D. H. Brainard,
“The psychophysics toolbox,”
Spat. Vis., 10
(4), 433
–436 https://doi.org/10.1163/156856897X00357 SPVIEU 0169-1015
(1997).
Google Scholar
N. T. Nguyen et al.,
“Cerebral hemodynamic responses to the sensory conflict between visual and rotary vestibular stimuli: an analysis with a multichannel near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) system,”
Front. Hum. Neurosci., 14 125 https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2020.00125
(2020).
Google Scholar
D. Carius et al.,
“Hemodynamic response alteration as a function of task complexity and expertise: an fNIRS study in jugglers,”
Front. Hum. Neurosci., 10 126 https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00126
(2016).
Google Scholar
T. Hashimoto, Y. Minagawa-Kawai and S. Kojima,
“Motion illusion activates the visual motion area of the brain: a near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) study,”
Brain Res., 1077
(1), 116
–122 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2005.11.050 BRREAP 0006-8993
(2006).
Google Scholar
J. Mazziotta et al.,
“A probabilistic atlas and reference system for the human brain: International Consortium for Brain Mapping (ICBM),”
Philos. Trans. R. Soc. London. Ser. B Biol. Sci., 356
(1412), 1293
–1322 https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2001.0915
(2001).
Google Scholar
D. L. Collins et al.,
“Automatic 3D intersubject registration of MR volumetric data in standardized Talairach space,”
J. Comput. Assist. Tomogr., 18
(2), 192
–205 https://doi.org/10.1097/00004728-199403000-00005 JCATD5 0363-8715
(1994).
Google Scholar
A. C. Evans et al.,
“3D statistical neuroanatomical models from 305 MRI volumes,”
in IEEE Conf. Record Nucl. Sci. Symp. and Med. Imaging Conf.,
1813
–1817
(1993). https://doi.org/10.1109/NSSMIC.1993.373602 Google Scholar
D. W. Shattuck et al.,
“Construction of a 3D probabilistic atlas of human cortical structures,”
Neuroimage, 39
(3), 1064
–1080 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.09.031 NEIMEF 1053-8119
(2008).
Google Scholar
R. H. Cai et al.,
“Vestibular signals can distort the perceived spatial relationship of retinal stimuli,”
Exp. Brain Res., 135
(2), 275
–278 https://doi.org/10.1007/s002210000549 EXBRAP 0014-4819
(2000).
Google Scholar
C. R. L. Cantor and C. M. Schor,
“Stimulus dependence of the flash-lag effect,”
Vis. Res., 47
(22), 2841
–2854 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2007.06.023
(2007).
Google Scholar
D. M. Eagleman and T. J. Sejnowski,
“The position of moving objects,”
Science, 289
(5482), 1107a https://doi.org/10.1126/science.289.5482.1107a SCIEAS 0036-8075
(2000).
Google Scholar
B. Krekelberg and M. Lappe,
“Temporal recruitment along the trajectory of moving objects and the perception of position,”
Vis. Res., 39
(16), 2669
–2679 https://doi.org/10.1016/S0042-6989(98)00287-9
(1999).
Google Scholar
M. Lappe and B. Krekelberg,
“The position of moving objects,”
Perception, 27
(12), 1437
–1449 https://doi.org/10.1068/p271437 PCTNBA 0301-0066
(1998).
Google Scholar
T. L. Hubbard,
“The flash-lag effect and related mislocalizations: findings, properties, and theories,”
Psychol. Bull., 140
(1), 308
–338 https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032899 PSBUAI 0033-2909
(2014).
Google Scholar
X. Hou et al.,
“NIRS-KIT: a MATLAB toolbox for both resting-state and task fNIRS data analysis,”
Neurophotonics, 8
(1), 010802 https://doi.org/10.1117/1.NPh.8.1.010802
(2021).
Google Scholar
K. J. Friston et al., Statistical Parametric Mapping: The Analysis of Functional Brain Images, Academic Press, Cambridge
(2007). Google Scholar
F. A. Fishburn et al.,
“Temporal derivative distribution repair (TDDR): a motion correction method for fNIRS,”
Neuroimage, 184 171
–179 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.09.025 NEIMEF 1053-8119
(2019).
Google Scholar
R. Huang et al.,
“Motion artifacts removal and evaluation techniques for functional near-infrared spectroscopy signals: a review,”
Front. Neurosci., 16 878750 https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2022.878750 1662-453X
(2022).
Google Scholar
C. P. Hoyniak et al.,
“Adversity is linked with decreased parent-child behavioral and neural synchrony,”
Dev. Cogn. Neurosci., 48 100937 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2021.100937
(2021).
Google Scholar
F. A. Fishburn et al.,
“Irritability uniquely predicts prefrontal cortex activation during preschool inhibitory control among all temperament domains: a LASSO approach,”
Neuroimage, 184 68
–77 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.09.023 NEIMEF 1053-8119
(2019).
Google Scholar
M. A. Yücel et al.,
“Best practices for fNIRS publications,”
Neurophotonics, 8
(1), 012101 https://doi.org/10.1117/1.NPh.8.1.012101
(2021).
Google Scholar
M. Hubert and E. Vandervieren,
“An adjusted boxplot for skewed distributions,”
Comput. Stat. Data Anal., 52
(12), 5186
–5201 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.csda.2007.11.008 CSDADW 0167-9473
(2008).
Google Scholar
S. Verboven and M. Hubert,
“LIBRA: a MATLAB library for robust analysis,”
Chemom. Intell. Lab. Syst., 75
(2), 127
–136 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chemolab.2004.06.003
(2005).
Google Scholar
S. Verboven and M. Hubert,
“MATLAB library LIBRA,”
Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Comput. Stat., 2
(4), 509
–515 https://doi.org/10.1002/wics.96
(2010).
Google Scholar
Y. Hoshi, N. Kobayashi and M. Tamura,
“Interpretation of near-infrared spectroscopy signals: a study with a newly developed perfused rat brain model,”
J. Appl. Physiol., 90
(5), 1657
–1662 https://doi.org/10.1152/jappl.2001.90.5.1657
(2001).
Google Scholar
G. Strangman et al.,
“A quantitative comparison of simultaneous BOLD fMRI and NIRS recordings during functional brain activation,”
Neuroimage, 17
(2), 719
–731 https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1227 NEIMEF 1053-8119
(2002).
Google Scholar
M. Wolf et al.,
“Different time evolution of oxyhemoglobin and deoxyhemoglobin concentration changes in the visual and motor cortices during functional stimulation: a near-infrared spectroscopy study,”
Neuroimage, 16
(3), 704
–712 https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1128 NEIMEF 1053-8119
(2002).
Google Scholar
L. Chen et al.,
“Distinct contributions of genes and environment to visual size illusion and the underlying neural mechanism,”
Cereb. Cortex, 32
(5), 1014
–1023 https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhab262 53OPAV 1047-3211
(2022).
Google Scholar
S. Bae, Y. Lee and P. Chang,
“There is no test–retest reliability of brain activation induced by robotic passive hand movement: a functional NIRS study,”
Brain Behav., 10
(10), e01788 https://doi.org/10.1002/brb3.1788
(2020).
Google Scholar
H. Takakura et al.,
“Cerebral hemodynamic responses during dynamic posturography: analysis with a multichannel near-infrared spectroscopy system,”
Front. Hum. Neurosci., 9 620 https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00620
(2015).
Google Scholar
Y. Benjamini and Y. Hochberg,
“Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing,”
J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B, 57
(1), 289
–300 https://doi.org/10.2307/2346101 JSTBAJ 0035-9246
(1995).
Google Scholar
A. H. Wertheim and G. Reymond,
“Neural noise distorts perceived motion: the special case of the freezing illusion and the Pavard and Berthoz effect,”
Exp. Brain Res., 180
(3), 569
–576 https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-007-0887-2 EXBRAP 0014-4819
(2007).
Google Scholar
A. A. Faisal, L. P. J. Selen and D. M. Wolpert,
“Noise in the nervous system,”
Nat. Rev. Neurosci., 9
(4), 292
–303 https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2258 NRNAAN 1471-003X
(2008).
Google Scholar
A. C. Huk, R. F. Dougherty and D. J. Heeger,
“Retinotopy and functional subdivision of human areas MT and MST,”
J. Neurosci., 22
(16), 7195
–7205 https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-16-07195.2002 JNRSDS 0270-6474
(2002).
Google Scholar
A. Malikovic et al.,
“Cytoarchitectonic analysis of the human extrastriate cortex in the region of V5/MT+: a probabilistic, stereotaxic map of area hOc5,”
Cereb. Cortex, 17
(3), 562
–574 https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhj181 53OPAV 1047-3211
(2006).
Google Scholar
H. Kolster, R. Peeters and G. A. Orban,
“The retinotopic organization of the human middle temporal area MT/V5 and its cortical neighbors,”
J. Neurosci., 30
(29), 9801
–9820 https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2069-10.2010 JNRSDS 0270-6474
(2010).
Google Scholar
B. Wild and S. Treue,
“Primate extrastriate cortical area MST: a gateway between sensation and cognition,”
J. Neurophysiol., 125
(5), 1851
–1882 https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00384.2020 JONEA4 0022-3077
(2021).
Google Scholar
W. K. Page and C. J. Duffy,
“Heading representation in MST: sensory interactions and population encoding,”
J. Neurophysiol., 89
(4), 1994
–2013 https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00493.2002 JONEA4 0022-3077
(2003).
Google Scholar
C. J. Duffy,
“MST neurons respond to optic flow and translational movement,”
J. Neurophysiol., 80
(4), 1816
–1827 https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1998.80.4.1816 JONEA4 0022-3077
(1998).
Google Scholar
Y. Gu et al.,
“Visual and nonvisual contributions to three-dimensional heading selectivity in the medial superior temporal area,”
J. Neurosci., 26
(1), 73
–85 https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2356-05.2006 JNRSDS 0270-6474
(2006).
Google Scholar
Y. Gu, G. C. DeAngelis and D. E. Angelaki,
“A functional link between area MSTd and heading perception based on vestibular signals,”
Nat. Neurosci., 10
(8), 1038
–1047 https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1935 NANEFN 1097-6256
(2007).
Google Scholar
Y. Gu, D. E. Angelaki and G. C. DeAngelis,
“Neural correlates of multisensory cue integration in macaque MSTd,”
Nat. Neurosci., 11
(10), 1201
–1210 https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2191 NANEFN 1097-6256
(2008).
Google Scholar
K. H. Britten and R. J. A. Van Wezel,
“Electrical microstimulation of cortical area MST biases heading perception in monkeys,”
Nat. Neurosci., 1
(1), 59
–63 https://doi.org/10.1038/259 NANEFN 1097-6256
(1998).
Google Scholar
T. Brandt et al.,
“Reciprocal inhibitory visual-vestibular interaction. Visual motion stimulation deactivates the parieto-insular vestibular cortex,”
Brain, 121
(9), 1749
–1758 https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/121.9.1749 BRAIAK 0006-8950
(1998).
Google Scholar
J. Conrad et al.,
“Recovery from spatial neglect with intra- and transhemispheric functional connectivity changes in vestibular and visual cortex areas: a case study,”
Front. Neurol., 9 112 https://doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2018.00112
(2018).
Google Scholar
S. Bense et al.,
“Metabolic changes in vestibular and visual cortices in acute vestibular neuritis,”
Ann. Neurol., 56
(5), 624
–630 https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.20244
(2004).
Google Scholar
C. Lopez and O. Blanke,
“The thalamocortical vestibular system in animals and humans,”
Brain Res. Rev., 67
(1–2), 119
–146 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresrev.2010.12.002 BRERD2 0165-0173
(2011).
Google Scholar
A. M. Wirth et al.,
“White matter connectivity of the visual–vestibular cortex examined by diffusion-weighted imaging,”
Brain Connect., 8
(4), 235
–244 https://doi.org/10.1089/brain.2017.0544
(2018).
Google Scholar
F. Lacquaniti et al.,
“Visual gravitational motion and the vestibular system in humans,”
Front. Integr. Neurosci., 7 101 https://doi.org/10.3389/fnint.2013.00101
(2013).
Google Scholar
V. Kirsch et al.,
“Handedness-dependent functional organizational patterns within the bilateral vestibular cortical network revealed by fMRI connectivity based parcellation,”
Neuroimage, 178 224
–237 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.05.018 NEIMEF 1053-8119
(2018).
Google Scholar
C. Helmchen et al.,
“Increased brain responsivity to galvanic vestibular stimulation in bilateral vestibular failure,”
NeuroImage Clin., 24 101942 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101942
(2019).
Google Scholar
H. Karim et al.,
“Functional brain imaging of multi-sensory vestibular processing during computerized dynamic posturography using near-infrared spectroscopy,”
Neuroimage, 74 318
–325 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.02.010 NEIMEF 1053-8119
(2013).
Google Scholar
F. Fiori et al.,
“The right temporoparietal junction plays a causal role in maintaining the internal representation of verticality,”
J. Neurophysiol., 114
(5), 2983
–2990 https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00289.2015 JONEA4 0022-3077
(2015).
Google Scholar
R. W. Sperry,
“Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visual inversion,”
J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol., 43
(6), 482
–489 https://doi.org/10.1037/h0055479 JCPPAV 0021-9940
(1950).
Google Scholar
E. von Holst and H. Mittelstaedt,
“Das Reafferenzprinzip,”
Naturwissenschaften, 37
(20), 464
–476 https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00622503 NATWAY 0028-1042
(1950).
Google Scholar
D. Subramanian, A. Alers and M. A. Sommer,
“Corollary discharge for action and cognition,”
Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging, 4
(9), 782
–790 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2019.05.010
(2019).
Google Scholar
S. Becker-Bense et al.,
“Direct comparison of activation maps during galvanic vestibular stimulation: a hybrid H2[15 O] PET—BOLD MRI activation study,”
PLoS One, 15
(5), e0233262 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0233262 POLNCL 1932-6203
(2020).
Google Scholar
M. Dieterich, V. Kirsch and T. Brandt,
“Right-sided dominance of the bilateral vestibular system in the upper brainstem and thalamus,”
J. Neurol., 264
(S1), 55
–62 https://doi.org/10.1007/s00415-017-8453-8
(2017).
Google Scholar
C. Lopez, O. Blanke and F. W. Mast,
“The human vestibular cortex revealed by coordinate-based activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis,”
Neuroscience, 212 159
–179 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.03.028
(2012).
Google Scholar
J. W. Philbeck et al.,
“Asymmetrical perception of body rotation after unilateral injury to human vestibular cortex,”
Neuropsychologia, 44
(10), 1878
–1890 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.02.004 NUPSA6 0028-3932
(2006).
Google Scholar
M. Dieterich,
“Dominance for vestibular cortical function in the non-dominant hemisphere,”
Cereb. Cortex, 13
(9), 994
–1007 https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/13.9.994 53OPAV 1047-3211
(2003).
Google Scholar
Q. Arshad et al.,
“Left cathodal trans-cranial direct current stimulation of the parietal cortex leads to an asymmetrical modulation of the vestibular-ocular reflex,”
Brain Stimul., 7
(1), 85
–91 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2013.07.002
(2014).
Google Scholar
W. O. Guldin and O.-J. Grüsser,
“Is there a vestibular cortex?,”
Trends Neurosci., 21
(6), 254
–259 https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-2236(97)01211-3 TNSCDR 0166-2236
(1998).
Google Scholar
Z. Cheng and Y. Gu,
“Vestibular system and self-motion,”
Front. Cell. Neurosci., 12 456 https://doi.org/10.3389/fncel.2018.00456
(2018).
Google Scholar
A. T. Smith et al.,
“Distributed visual–vestibular processing in the cerebral cortex of man and Macaque,”
Multisens. Res., 30
(2), 91
–120 https://doi.org/10.1163/22134808-00002568
(2017).
Google Scholar
S. L. Novi et al.,
“Integration of spatial information increases reproducibility in functional near-infrared spectroscopy,”
Front. Neurosci., 14 746 https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2020.00746 1662-453X
(2020).
Google Scholar
A. Machado et al.,
“Optimal positioning of optodes on the scalp for personalized functional near-infrared spectroscopy investigations,”
J. Neurosci. Methods, 309 91
–108 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2018.08.006 JNMEDT 0165-0270
(2018).
Google Scholar
F. Herold et al.,
“Applications of functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) neuroimaging in exercise–cognition science: a systematic, methodology-focused review,”
J. Clin. Med., 7
(12), 466 https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm7120466
(2018).
Google Scholar
K. N. de Winkel et al.,
“Neural correlates of decision making on whole body yaw rotation: an fNIRS study,”
Neurosci. Lett., 654 56
–62 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2017.04.053 NELED5 0304-3940
(2017).
Google Scholar
BiographyXin He received his PhD in cognitive neuroscience in 2021 from the Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences. His research interests include visual-vestibular interaction and the attentional effect on ocular dominance plasticity. Min Bao received his BA degree in biology and his PhD in cognitive psychology from the School of Life Sciences, University of Science and technology of China. In 2012, he established the Visual Perception and Plasticity Laboratory in the Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences. The research focuses of the lab include ocular dominance plasticity, visual adaptation, and visual-vestibular interactions, especially using altered or virtual reality. |
Visualization
Neuroimaging
Baryon acoustic oscillations
Brain
Neurophotonics
Signal to noise ratio
Head